Tourette Syndrome Life's a Twitch Logo


Writings - Research Papers

Life's A Twitch! ®


Home

Nix Your Tics

Documentary

Writings

Youth Haven

Ask Dr. Dunc

Accolades

Presentations

Resources

Links

Disclaimer

Contact


If you are a new visitor, diagnosed with a difference, please read this introduction letter to you.

For all other new visitors, Dr. McKinlay also has a special introduction letter to you.


Nix Your Tics! Front Cover image

Nix Your Tics!

The Second
(E-)dition from "Life's A Twitch! Publishing". Click here to learn more.


To watch the Life's A Twitch! documentary via streaming video, click here.


If you would like to reprint writings from this site, please click here.


Before Signing the Guestbook

Guest Book Icon


Nix Your Tics Facebook Group Nix Your Tics Twitter Feed


If you wish to return to the 'Research Papers' archive, please click here.

If you would like to reprint this paper, please click here.


Life's A Twitch! Celebrating 15 years.
1998 - 2018
Life's A Twitch! Celebrating 15 years.

 

Schizophrenia: Grandaddy Disinhibition?


Many professionals characterize Schizophrenia as a disorder which is too bizarre or alien for the average individual to empathize with, and certainly as separate from other disorders in it's severity of expression and debilitation of it's victims (Bellack and Hersen 1993, Lehrmann 1955, Modrow 1995). Similarly, Tourette's Syndrome, and it's associated symptoms, are considered "bizarre" (Blakeslee, 1996), "strange" (Sacks, 1995), and "crazy" (Yelaja, 1997). This paper will discuss current theory which links Tourette's Syndrome and various comorbid conditions to a common etiology, and will explore the possibility that even Schizophrenia may fit within this new perspective of many mental disorders.

Tourette Syndrome, until recently, was considered to be a dominant-gene disorder with relative scarcity within the population - about 1 in 20,000 (Comings, 1990). Recent concordance studies and pedigree analyses (Comings, 1990) have suggested that Tourette's Syndrome is but one manifestation of a Generalized Disinhibition Disorder, caused by a semi-dominant, semi-recessive gene or genes. While this predisposition to be disinhibited may manifest as Tourette's Syndrome, it may also appear in other motoric guises (motor hyperactivity, chronic tics), as frontal lobe dysfunction (ADHD, Conduct Disorder), in learning and memory (LD, dyslexia), as anxiety (phobias, panic, GAD), or in obsessive-compulsive tendencies (substance abuse, obesity, OCD) (Pauls, Towbin & Leckman 1986, Cumings & Frankel 1985, Green & Pitman 1986). Currently, TS is thought to be the manifestation of two of these Gts genes; individuals carrying only one Gts gene "may either have no symptoms or one or more of the behaviours listed above" (Comings, 1990). This would explain why the comorbidity of such disorders as ADHD and OCD with TS are much higher than the comorbidity of TS with them (Comings, 1990).

There are many similarities between Schizophrenia and this hypothesized group of "Disinhibition Disorders". Schizophrenia, like the disinhibition disorders, is believed to be genetic, and both are postulated to be genetically additive (Comings, 1990, Bellack & Hersen, 1993). Emil Kraepelin, hailed as the "Father of Modern Psychiatry" and who first discriminated dementia praecox from manic depression, noted in his extensive observations of Schizophrenics that many afflicted had odd mannerisms and/or movement disorders. It is also interesting that Tardive Dyskinesia, a movement disorder resembling Tourette's Syndrome, develops after prolonged usage of anti-psychotic medications such as Haldol (Sandor, 1995). Tardive Dyskinesia can also occur in the treatment of TS with the same drugs (Sandor, 1995), and in general it is not unusual for the symptoms of a different disinhibition disorder to develop during medication of the current disinhibition disorder (such as tics appearing during the treatment of ADHD or OCD increasing in the treatment of TS - Greenhill, 1989). Attentional and distraction problems are also cited as being associated with Schizophrenia - either as a focal symptom (McGhie & Chapman in Buss & Buss, 1969) or as associated ones (APA, 1994). Early Behaviourists suggested that the loose associations in Schizophrenia are actually a learned response to avoid anxiety (Mednick in Buss & Buss, 1969) - in effect, a severe attention deficit is self-trained to the extent that the individual becomes schizophrenic. Fully remitted schizophrenics describe intense obsessions and intrusive thoughtsthat literally never stop for 24 hours a day (Modrow, 1995), and research shows that the symptoms of OCD and schizophrenia do co-occur (Bellack & Hersen, 1993). The presence of OCD and psychotic-like symptoms oftentimes indicate the presence of a schizotypal personality style (Bellack & Hersen, 1993), which is considered by some to be the precursor to Schizophrenia (Meehl in Buss & Buss, 1969) and used to be the Simple subtype of Schizophrenia (Davison & Neale, 1994).

With regards to physiology, there are many similarities between Schizophrenia and disinhibition disorders as well. Enlarged ventricles are typical of process schizophrenics (Frith, 1992) movement disorders (Owens et. al., 1985, in Frith, 1992), and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (Bellack & Hersen, 1993). The striatum (a component of the basal ganglia) is implicated in Schizophrenia (Frith, 1995), TS (Comings, 1990), and OCD (Bellack & Hersen, 1993). Frontal lobe dysfunction in recent years has become an area of research in Schizophrenia (Bornstein, 1991). The frontal lobes are involved in judgment, rationality, and critical thinking, and it has been discovered that in Schizophrenia the left frontal cortex shows much less metabolism during activity (Frith, 1995). Left frontal underactivity has also been identified in OCD (Bellack & Hersen, 1993), and TS (Doran, 1996); frontal "brown-out" is also a hallmark of ADHD (Doran 1996; Kronenberger & Meyer 1996). Abnormal catecholamine levels are involved with ADHD, OCD, TS, and Schizophrenia (Comings, 1990, APA, 1994) and dopamine specifically is discussed in Schizophrenia (Frith, 1992) and TS (Comings, 1990). In sum, it seems that problems with the connections and communications between the striatum (which is influenced by dopamine levels, and is involved in the willing and execution of movement) and the prefrontal cortex (the "goal-centers") cause the symptoms of Schizophrenia (Frith, 1992) including the obsessional components and perseverances also seen in TS.

The prognoses of Schizophrenia, TS, ADHD, and OCD parallel one another - all can be chronic, lifelong conditions, or can completely remit. It is only in the past few years that ADHD in adulthood has reached wide acknowledgment (Kronenberger & Meyer, 1996), and a portion of the TS population does remit in their early 20's (Comings, 1990). As the fundamental problem with being overly disinhibited is a high level of stimulation, environmental stressors have a significant impact on the course of disinhibition disorders. Likewise, meta-analyses have shown that disturbed and distressing parental interactions can trigger and influence the expression of Schizophrenia (Goldstein & Strachen in Vinogradov, 1995). The negativism and immobility in catatonic schizophrenia that suddenly erupts into violent outbursts (Davison & Neale, 1994) is reminiscent of ADHD and Tourettic rage when stimulation thresholds are reached (Pruitt, 1996).

Despite all of the overlap between various disinhibition disorders, and between Schizophrenia and disinhibition disorders, there are still obvious differences. Schizophrenia is much more severe than TS, ADHD and OCD. Onset of schizophrenia tends to be much later - early 20's for men to early 30's for women (Jablensky, 1992) versus four for ADHD (Kronenberger & Meyer, 1996), seven for TS (Comings, 1990), and late adolescence for OCD (Bellack & Hersen, 1993).

 

How then do we conceptualize the overlap of these various conditions? Just as the various and diverse forms of Schizophrenia are believed to be bound by the fundamental and primary feature of loosening of associations (Bleuler, in Arieti, 1955), all of these conditions are different manifestations of the common process of disinhibition. TS can be conceptualized as a motoric disinhibition, ADHD as a disinhibition of the focusing of attention, and OCD as a disinhibition of specific concerns, wishes, and/or cognitions. Schizophrenia, then, is a disinhibition of thinking in a generalized sense - rather than experiencing a specific enervation or stimulation in a localized area of the brain (a motoric centre, or particular ruminations) causing repetition and attendance to a particular ritual or thought, a more generalized overstimulation occurs. The result is that ALL cognitions are in a state of arousal. While in this heightened state, it would take little additional stimulation for each association to reach threshold for awareness, and odd associations between unrelated cognitions would be forged due simply to the fact that both are simultaneously aroused (Hebb, 1949). Thus the individual's attention would bound from association to association, no matter how tangentially related; while a normally inhibited individual can remain coherent and "on-track" because the next logical associative thought is stimulated to a greater degree than distantly related associations, this differential is lost in individuals with Schizophrenia.

Illustratively, imagine walking a dog through a cat-show on a leash. The dog very much wants to "associate" with ALL of these various felines, however it will remain to "associate" with it's master, as the leash's strength is stronger than the allure of the cats. Thus the dog will continue on a progressive and planned course throughout the cat-show with its master. What if suddenly the leash is cut (i.e., the differential strengths of associations is eliminated)? Of course the dog will tear off hysterically after the first cat it sees, and if another, closer cat catches it's attention it will abruptly shift course and aim for the new target, until another grabs it's eye, and another, and another. Obviously the result is chaos.

 

Frith (1992) postulated a model of Schizophrenia in which symptoms are the result of a failure to self-monitor. Positive symptoms (i.e. hallucinations and delusions) occur when a willed action is carried out by the basal ganglia, but the latter fails to send a feedback message to the perceptual center. In other words, an individual will "self-talk" or initiate a movement but will not be AWARE that (s)he is/did. Hallucinations are the interpretations of "self-talk" in the absence of self-awareness, and delusions (such as delusions of control) are the interpretations of movements without the knowledge that (s)he planned them (Frith, 1992). Negative symptoms are considered to be even heavier damage to communications between the cortex, basal ganglia, and the perceptual centers - wills are no longer able to generate intentions to act at all.

This theory requires only slight modification to fit the disinhibition model. Positive symptoms will be addressed first. Assuming that the basal ganglia is designed to deliver a "normal" number of feedback messages to the perceptual system, in a state of generalized disinhibition (overstimulation), it may be that the system either "shorts out", or continues to work but "misses" some messages. These "missed" messages are then the ones attributed as hallucinations and delusions. Frisk (1992) also suggested that positive symptoms could be the result of stimulus-driven actions (afforded actions) that are not inhibited by one's goals (communication problems between the prefrontal cortex and the striatum). This is more or less synonymous with the definition of TS.

With regards to negative symptoms, under a state of extreme stimulation one is less likely to initiate actions at all - despite a will to involve oneself in life in various ways, overstimulation deters one from developing an intention. Recall Hans Eysenck's theory on introversion-extroversion: introverts do not need to seek sensation (or initiate actions) to the same degree as do extroverts, as their internal stimulation is already high (Day, 1992). Individuals with disinhibition disorders (ex. TS) sometimes will avoid social contact in an attempt to minimize stimulation levels.

Modifying Frith's (1992) theories of schizophrenia so that they are framed within the context of disinhibition helps to explain certain facts that were inconsistent with Frith's original conceptions. First, there is considerable research that suggests that even the most severely debilitated of schizophrenics can recover - some undergo complete remission (Modrow, 1995). If there were actual structural lesions in schizophrenia this would not be possible (although it is certainly possible that after years of overstimulation certain irreversible structural damages could occur, thus rendering the condition permanent). There have also been reports of professionals being treated after-hours for schizophrenia, and incidents where seemingly completely debilitated schizophrenics have somehow "suppressed" their symptoms long enough to operate normally for some time before returning to their debilitated state (Modrow, 1995). Hyperfocusing in ADHD (Doran, 1996) and temporary suppression of tics in TS (Comings, 1990) are well-documented and analogous phenomena.

Second, if there were actual mechanical problems with the relay of messages why do dopamine-blocking drugs completely eliminate hallucinations and delusions? Frith (1992) suggested that dopamine-blockers operate indirectly; by tranquilizing the individual globally, one reduces the number of movements that could be misattributed to hallucinations and delusions because one reduces the overall number of movements. With a structural-problem hypothesis, one would REDUCE hallucinations and delusions, but as long as the individual made ANY movement hallucinations and delusions would still exist. Within the disinhibition model, however, this inconsistency is eliminated - by reducing the amount of stimulation, and hence number of messages, impinging on the basal ganglia to "normal" levels, the system is no longer overtaxed, it resumes "normal" activity, and no messages are "missed" (and misattributed) anymore.

Finally, it is difficult to explain how stimulants can mimic Schizophrenia if structural problems are definitive of the disorder (as in Amphetamine Psychosis - APA, 1994), but it makes perfect sense within a disinhibition model - in "natural" schizophrenics, overstimulation is caused by disinhibition. In "amphetamine" schizophrenics the overstimulation is artificial. Regardless of the locus of the overstimulation, the effects are the same.

In sum, Frith's (1992) theory that schizophrenic symptoms are caused by a problem in self-monitoring seems valid. It is interesting to note that one popular method of treating individuals with TS is to explicitly teach self-monitoring strategies (Kronenberger & Meyer, 1996). Frith's reasons for the self-monitoring problems were questioned, however; the hypothesis that self-monitoring difficulties are the result of overstimulation due to disinhibition seems to fit the known facts better than the hypothesis that there is actual structural damage to the brain (Frith, 1992).

 

If Schizophrenia IS to be considered as a disinhibition disorder, it is obviously the most debilitating and severe of them all. While the predominant feature of Schizophrenia has been outlined to be the disinhibition of general thought associations, it seems to have characteristics of many other disinhibition disorders such as ADHD, TS, and OCD (as described earlier). The idea that genes could work additively was previously mentioned - it may be that, just as TS is considered by some to be "ADHD + tics" (Doran, 1996), Schizophrenia is "ADHD, OCD, TS, etc. + thought disinhibitions". Genetic heterogeneity (Bellack & Hersen, 1993) is the current genetic theory of Schizophrenia - it suggests that while there is an additive genetic component to Schizophrenia, there are one or two dominant Schizophrenia genes which are necessary and sufficient for the disorder (Bellack & Hersen, 1993). Perhaps there does exist a particular gene that causes the disinhibition of thought, and these "additive genes" are other disinhibition, or Gts, genes. It does not seem unreasonable to suspect that many genes exist whose functions involve a wide range of disinhibitory functions - disinhibition in general is essential to survival in order to manipulate the environment, and certain disinhibitions (for example, sexual) have been postulated to have particular adaptive advantage (Comings, 1990). Considering that the human cortex has evolved to be inhibitory, the need for SOMETHING to cause disinhibition is of course essential.

After having developed dimensions of disinhibition, the next theoretical step in such a discussion seems to be to discuss spectrums of disinhibition. Are all of these different disinhibition disorders, including Schizophrenia, merely different manifestations of the same amount of overstimulation, or are some disorders characterized by more disinhibition than others? One might tentatively postulate a spectrum such as:

Extrovert -- Introvert -- ADHD -- TS/OCD -- Reactive Sx -- Process Sx

This spectrum would seem to apply not only to degree of internal disinhibition, but also to age of onset [Infants as young as 22 months can be categorized by temperament (Plomin & Rowe 1979 in Day, 1992)], and prognosis.

 

In conclusion, the evidence supporting the consideration of Schizophrenia as a disinhibition disorder seems persuasive and plentiful. Current neurophysiological models of Schizophrenia can be modified to reflect overstimulation of thought associations as the fundamental cause of both positive and negative symptoms; in this altered form these models can account for more findings in the literature than they could without incorporating the idea of disinhibition. Schizophrenia may fit on a spectrum of disinhibition, in which it is the accumulation of an assortment of other disinhibition disorders and, thus, the most severe of all subtypes.


References

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association.

Arieti, Silvano (1955). Interpretation of Schizophrenia. New York: Robert Brunner.

Bellack, Alan S., and Michel Hersen (1993). Psychopathology in Adulthood. Toronto: Allyn and Bacon.

Blakeslee, Sandra (1996). Faulty neurological brakes may cause Tourette's tics. The Globe and Mail, Sept. 21.

Bornstein, R.A. (1991). Neuropsychological correlates of obsessive characteristics in Tourette Syndrome. Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 3, pgs. 157-162.

Buss, Arnold H. and Edith H. Buss (eds.) (1969). Theories of Schizophrenia. New York: Atherton Press.

Comings, David E. (1990). Tourette Syndrome and Human Behaviour. California: Hope Press.

Cumings, JL, and Frankel, M. (1985). Gilles de la Tourette syndrome and the neurological basis of obsessions and compulsions. Biological Psychology, 20, pgs. 1117-1126.

Davison, Gerald C., and John M. Neale. Abnormal Psychology. 6th ed. Toronto: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Day, Richard B. (1992). Theories of Personality Student Handbook. Massachusetts: Ginn Press.

Diamond, M.C., Scheibel, A.B., and L.M. Elson (1985). The Human Brain Coloring Book. New York: HarperCollins Inc.

Doran, Mort (August 1996). Tourette Syndrome Presentation. Chedoke-McMaster Hospital, Hamilton, ON.

Doran, Mort (Jan. 23, 1997). Tourette Syndrome Presentation. Kitchener-Waterloo Board of Education, Kitchener-Waterloo, ON.

Frith, Christopher D. (1992). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Kronenberger, William G., and Robert G. Meyer (1996). The Child Clinician's Handbook. Toronto: Allyn and Bacon.

Green, RC, and Pitman, RK (1986). Tourette Syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder, in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorders: Therapy and Management. Edited by Jenike, MA et. al. PSG, pgs. 147-164.

Greenhill, L.L. (1989). Pharmacologic treatment of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Pediatric Psychopharmacology, 15, pgs. 1-27.

Hebb, DO (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.

Jablensky, A. et. al. (1992). Schizophrenia: manifestations, incidence and course in different cultures A World Health Organization ten-country study. Psychological Medicine (Monograph Supplement 20). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Lehrmann, Heinz (1955). National Film Board: Series on Schizophrenia.

Modrow, John (1995). How To Become A Schizophrenic The Case Against Biological Psychiatry. 2nd ed. Washington: Apollyon Press.

Owens, D.G.C., Johnstone, E.C., Crow, T.J., et. al. (1985). Lateral ventricle size in schizophrenia: relation to the disease process and its clinical manifestations. Psychological Medicine, 15, pgs. 27-41.

Pauls, DL, Towbin, KE, Leckman, JF, et. al. (1986) Gilles de la Tourette syndrome and obsessive compulsive disorder: evidence supporting an etiological relationship. Archive of General Psychology, 43, pgs. 1180-1182.

Pruitt, Sheryl K (1996). Teacher's Workshop on Tourette syndrome, Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Toronto, ON.

Sacks, Oliver (1995). The surgeon who ticced. The Globe and Mail. March 4.

Sandor, P. (1995). Clinical management of Tourette's syndrome and associated disorders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 40, pgs. 577-583.

Vinogradov, Sophia and Irvin D. Yalom (eds.) (1995). Treating Schizophrenia. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers.

Yelaja, Priti (1997). Tourette torment. The Record. Jan. 24.

Top of Page


Home

Nix Your Tics!

Documentary

Writings

Youth Haven Ask Dr. Dunc

Accolades

Presentations

Resources

Links

Disclaimer Contact

http://www.lifesatwitch.com/sx.html
Last updated on March 25, 2022

© 1998 - 2020.  Life's A Twitch!® & design are registered trademarks of B. Duncan McKinlay, Ph.D., C.Psych. (Retired)

All activities related to Life's A Twitch!® are conducted by B. Duncan McKinlay, Ph.D., C.Psych. (Retired) in a private capacity and do not represent the Ministry of Children and Youth Services or the Government of Ontario.

Dr. B. Duncan McKinlay's Life's A Twitch!® Site on Tourette Syndrome & Associated Disorders

d